Tag Archives: parrots

Humans and Cockatoos Are in an ‘Arms Race’ Over Trash in Sydney

A cockatoo trying to push off the brick placed to keep it away from the trash it craves.
Photo: Barbara Klump

In Sydney, Australia, man and bird are waging a fierce battle over the most precious of resources: garbage. Over the past several years, a team of scientists has studied sulphur-crested cockatoo parrots in the area that have learned—and even taught other parrots—how to rob garbage bins. And in new research Monday, the team says that humans have now started to devise their own methods to keep the birds out, to varying degrees of success.

Researchers at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior in Germany have long been interested in deciphering the inner workings of animals around the world. Last year, they published a deep dive into the trash-robbing habits of Sydney’s sulphur-crested cockatoos. They found that the practice seemed to be an example of animal culture: a learned behavior that spread from birds in three suburbs to throughout Southern Sydney. As the technique passed from neighborhood to neighborhood, the local cockatoos developed slight variations to the behavior, such as lifting the bin lid entirely open or not—something that happens commonly enough in human culture (think about how different local cultures produce their own varieties of cheese).

The researchers told Gizmodo last year that they were next interested in documenting the human side of this struggle. And that’s just what they’ve done in their new paper, published Monday in the journal Current Biology.

Photo: Barbara Klump

“When we collected data for the original study describing bin-opening behavior by cockatoos, I saw that some people had put devices on their bins to protect them against cockatoos, and I was surprised by the variety of different measures that people had come up with. So I really wanted to investigate the human response to the cockatoos,” lead author Barbara Klump, a behavioral ecologist at the Max Planck Institute, told Gizmodo in an email.

To do so, they surveyed people living in neighborhoods beleaguered by these birds. A major stumbling block to any potential anti-cockatoo trick is that the bins are designed to open and spill their contents when lifted by the automated arm on garbage trucks, meaning they can’t be kept entirely sealed tight. But that hasn’t stopped people from devising a variety of methods, like putting bricks and stones onto the lids, fastening water bottles to the lid handles with cable ties, or using sticks to block the hinges. There are now even commercially available locks that are supposed to unlatch come collection time (one such product can be seen here).

Unfortunately for the humans, cockatoos have learned how to defeat some of the simpler measures. But much as the birds are adapting, people are developing counters right back. As the researchers put it, the parrots and people of Sydney seem to be engaged in a sort of innovation “arms race,” though Klump balked at describing it as a full-on war.

“When cockatoos learn to defeat this protection measure (e.g. by pushing off bricks so that they can then open the bin), people in our survey have reported that they increase the efficacy of their protection measures (e.g. by fixing something heavy to the lid, so that it cannot be pushed off). What we have found is that bin protection (and protection types) are geographically clustered and that people learn about them from their neighbors,” Klump said.

The entire saga, the researchers say, may be a preview of the sort of increasingly common interactions between people and wildlife that we can expect as we continue to build our cities larger and encroach on wildlife habitats. Some animals, like these parrots, may find new ways to adapt to our presence, yet many others won’t. And sometimes, these interactions can be harmful to humans, such as with the emergence of new zoonotic infectious diseases.

What exactly will happen next is anyone’s guess. “One could imagine that it will continue to escalate (i.e. cockatoos learning to defeat higher-level protection types, and people coming up with even better devices to protect their bins) or it could be that one party ‘wins’ the arms race,” Klump said.

For their part, the team plans to keep studying the underlying learning mechanisms that led these cockatoos to become proficient trash collectors, and they hope to document how adept they might become at solving the latest countermeasures meant to keep them from their garbage treasure.

Read original article here

Heidi Fleiss vows to leave Nevada after parrot shot: Report

Infamous Hollywood madam Heidi Fleiss has vowed to leave her Nevada home after someone shot one of her parrots, according to a report.

Fleiss, known for the prostitution ring she once ran in Los Angeles, claims someone injured one of her macaws with a shot from a pellet gun right before Christmas, and she is now offering $5,000 for information helping arrest the culprit.

The former madam, who counted Charlie Sheen as a client and once served 20 months in federal prison for tax evasion, has lived in Pahrump, about an hour west of Las Vegas, for 15 years.

The bird, named Chuey, is five years old, Fleiss told the Pahrump Valley Times, adding she kept the pellet a vet removed from her bird’s leg as evidence.

“Everyone knows they’re my birds and they really enjoy them,” Fleiss told the outlet. “When [the bird] came home in the late afternoon, she fell and that’s when I noticed there was something wrong with her leg.”

She vowed revenge on the fowl fiend who injured her pet.

Heidi Fleiss has lived in her home in Pahrump, Nevada for 15 years.
Roger Kisby

“I want to castrate him, I want them to get the death penalty for shooting my little Chuey, who was just flying around doing nothing — and they shoot her,” she told the paper. “I want them to have the death penalty because everyone in the subdivision knows her and they love her.”

So now she’s flocking to Missouri, where Fleiss, 56, bought a 50-acre forest for herself and her feathered friends.

“My birds will be out of here by the end of February. I think the best moments of my life have been here with my birds, watching them fly and explore and seeing them have freedom from years of sitting in cages,” she said. “It’s really been an incredible experience and I’ve had some of the best times of my life here — but unfortunately, the worst.”

Read original article here

Wild Parrots in Australia Teach Each Other to Break Into Trash Bins

One of the trash-loving parrots at work in Sydney, Australia.
Photo: Credit Barbara Klump/Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior

Scientists have discovered the latest fad sweeping across the sulphur-crested cockatoo parrots of Sydney, Australia: lifting up trash can lids to score a snack. In a new study this week, they detail the recent emergence and spread of this learned behavior, which they say is a common but not always easily observed example of cultural change happening among non-human animals.

Lucy Aplin and her team at the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior have long been interested in unraveling the social lives of animals, with a particular focus on birds. Their earlier research, for instance, has shown that great tit birds in the UK can quickly pick up and then pass on a method for solving a puzzle that would yield juicy mealworms—an aptitude for learning that might explain how these birds en masse raided the milk bottles of an English town a century earlier by breaking open the caps so they could steal the cream inside.

This time, Aplin and her team worked with other researchers in Australia to investigate recent sightings of sulphur-crested cockatoos, a native bird, breaking into trash bins across Sydney.

“We are very interested in understanding the potential role of the spread of innovation as a mechanism for behavioural flexibility in changing environments like cities, so when we first saw this new innovation in cockatoos, we knew we had to study if it was spreading via social learning,” Aplin, who heads the Cognitive and Cultural Ecology lab at Max Planck, told Gizmodo in an email.

Their new research, published Thursday in the journal Science, had several different angles to it. First, they surveyed people in various neighborhoods between 2018 and 2019 about whether they had seen the birds trash diving. Ultimately, they collected more than 300 sightings of trash bin lid lifting from 44 suburban neighborhoods, with most involving multiple parrots. Then they actually went out and tagged more than 400 cockatoos (with temporary color marking) found in three hotspots so they could observe the behavior themselves.

From all their work, they determined that, before 2018, trash bin lid lifting was likely only happening in three suburbs. But as this behavior began to spread, they found that it would subtly shift from place to place, essentially creating local flavors of lifting. Birds in one neighborhood might keep the lid propped up the whole time, as opposed to birds that completely flip the lid open, for instance. There were also clear patterns in who did the lifting, with males representing 84% of attempts. Birds of all ages lifted the lids, suggesting the behavior was passed through different groups in cockatoo society, but the most socially dominant males tended to be the most successful foragers, perhaps indicating that they had first dibs on the trash.

“Our study adds to the evidence that other animals have culture, and shows how new innovations can spread across populations to lead to new behaviors,” Aplin said.

Learned behaviors among socially adept animals have been documented many times before, such as with chimps passing along knowledge about tool use. In these sightings, there’s also been evidence of cultural diversity, with different groups of chimps adopting different variations of tool use. But according to Aplin, there’s been less work looking at how humans and the environments we make can directly shape animal culture, especially up close like this.

“These findings show that new cultures can develop rapidly in response to urban, human-provided opportunities, too,” she said.

While it’s possible that trash bin break-ins might become the hottest craze in cockatoo world, the behavior actually spread less rapidly than Aplin and her team figured it would. One possible reason for this delay might just be that it’s not exactly the easiest trick to learn, since it can take months for birds to get the hang of it. Natural barriers like forestland might impede its spread to other neighborhoods, as could the fact that male birds (as opposed to females) tend to stay close to home. City parrots also tend to migrate less, which could affect its popularity there compared to the suburbs. And of course, there’s always those meddling humans to worry about.

“People are beginning to protect bins, as they would understandably like to reduce the mess caused by cockatoos riffling through them!” Aplin noted. “We are really interested in following this human behaviour over time to see what effect that has on cockatoo behaviour.”

Whatever happens to these trash-loving birds, Aplin and her team hope that their research can further shine a light on how animals can culturally adapt to a changing world, just as humans have for millennia.

“Our capacity for innovation and culture is the secret to our success, allowing us to live in many different environments and adapt to many new situations. This work shows that this ability is not entirely limited to humans—some other animals have the capacity for rapid behavioural adaptation, too,” she said. “Anthropogenic change is rapid and ever increasing—understanding these behavioral responses to novel environments is vitally important if we want to understand when and how animals will cope with these changes.”

Read original article here

These ancient Americans mummified parrots. No one knows why

Parrots and macaws are not native to the Atacama, which is the driest desert in the world, but feathers and mummified birds have been found at archaeological sites in the region, according to a news release from Penn State University, published Monday.

Many parrots were mummified after death, some with their mouths open and their tongues sticking out, and others with their wings spread as if they were flying.

“It’s difficult to interpret,” study co-author José M. Capriles, assistant professor of anthropology at Penn State, told CNN, but the practice may be part of a ritual linked to the birds’ ability to mimic human speech.

Researchers visited museums throughout northern Chile for almost three years to study parrot and macaw remains found in the region. The scientists used zooarchaeological analysis, isotopic dietary reconstruction, radiocarbon dating and ancient DNA testing to build a picture of the birds’ lives.

The team found that the birds were brought to the Atacama from the Amazon, around 300 miles away, between 1100 and 1450 CE.

That time period saw a lot of commerce, with an increasing number of llama caravans moving between different parts of the Andes mountain range, Capriles told CNN.

“The fact that live birds made their way across the more-than-10,000-foot-high Andes is amazing,” Capriles said in a news statement. “They had to be transported across huge steppes, cold weather and difficult terrain to the Atacama. And they had to be kept alive.”

The birds’ arrival predated the Inca Empire and the Spanish colonization of the region, which brought horses to South America for the first time.

“Llamas are not the best pack animals, because they aren’t that strong,” Capriles said in the news release. “The fact that llama caravans brought macaws and parrots across the Andes and across the desert to this oasis is amazing.”

Once the birds arrived in the Atacama, they would have been kept as pets but also regularly plucked for their feathers, which were used in headdresses denoting wealth and power, Capriles told CNN.

The birds were fed the same food as the people who kept them, but their relationship with humans was complicated, he said.

“What we consider acceptable interactions with animals under our care was very different back then,” Capriles said. “Some of these birds did not live a happy life. They were kept to produce feathers and their feathers were plucked out as soon as they grew in.”

Many questions remain about the birds and the way they were used, and Capriles plans to continue his research in the region.

The paper was published in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

Read original article here