Tag Archives: ability

Vaccine Ability to Prevent Hospitalization From COVID-19 Still High

  • COVID-19 vaccine efficacy against infection seems to have declined, a CDC scientist said. 
  • Dr. Sara Oliver said vaccines are still highly effective at preventing hospitalization. 
  • “Vaccines remain effective in preventing hospitalization and severe disease,” Oliver said.

COVID-19 vaccines are still highly effective against hospitalization despite the surge in the more transmissible Delta variant, a scientist with the Centers for Disease Control said on Monday. 

In a presentation to the CDC’S Advisory Committee on Immunization Practices on Monday, Dr. Sara Oliver said that since Delta appeared, vaccine effectiveness against hospitalization ranged from 75% to 95%.

The vaccine’s ability to prevent infection ranged from 39% to 84% since the introduction of the Delta variant. 

“Vaccines remain effective in preventing hospitalization and severe disease but might be less effective in preventing infection or milder symptomatic illness,” the presentation said. 

The slides said both waning protection over time and the Delta variant may be contributing to the lower effectiveness of the vaccines. 

Oliver said it’s not uncommon for some vaccines to require multiple doses, including vaccines for hepatitis and HPV. However, she said more information is needed before the group can assess the need for booster shots. 

Dr. Anthony Fauci, director of the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases, previously said the pandemic can be brought under control by next spring if the millions of people not yet vaccinated get their shots. The presentation said the top priority should be to vaccinate those Americans. 

Experts have warned that the spread of the virus, specifically the Delta variant, could lead to a more dangerous variant.

“This is a very wily virus,” Fauci said. “If we keep lingering without getting those people vaccinated that should be vaccinated, this thing could linger on, leading to the development of another variant, which could complicate things.”

Those most at risk of severe disease should be prioritized for booster shots, the presentation added. 

Oliver also said it’s important to ensure there is global vaccine availability. “Uncontrolled spread globally that could result in new variants threaten control of the pandemic everywhere,” her presentation said. 

 

 

Loading Something is loading.

Read original article here

J.T. Tuimoloau Turning Heads at Ohio State Camp with Raw Ability, Work Ethic

Larry Johnson was out in his yard cutting his grass in early July when he felt his phone buzz.

The Ohio State defensive line coach looked down and saw a text from prized 2021 defensive line recruit J.T. Tuimoloau.

“Coach, I’m going to call you in about 20 minutes,” the text message read. 

Johnson moved to the steps on his porch as he tried to compose himself. So much effort had been put in from Johnson and other OSU staffers in recruiting Tuimoloau. The latest push to land the No. 4 recruit in the nation involved nearly every member of the Buckeyes’ coaching staff greeting the Sammamish, Washington resident at John Glenn Columbus International Airport on June 17 for an all-out official visit. At long last, OSU was going to get its answer on if the cherry on top of an already-strong 2021 class was coming. 

“I waited like a little kid,” Johnson said, detailing the anticipation of Tuimoloau’s phone call. He considers himself a patient man, but that day was truly a test of patience. 

Finally, the phone rang. 

“Coach, do you have a minute,” Tuimoloau asked. Johnson, an OSU assistant since 2014, responded in the affirmative. 

“We’ve decided what we’re going to do,” Tuimoloau told Johnson. The defensive line coach said he almost passed out from the sheer anticipation at that point. 

“You don’t know where that’s going to go,” Johnson said. “I don’t want bad news.” 

Fortunately for Johnson, any crisis was averted. “Coach, I’m going to be a Buckeye,” Tuimoloau said. 

Johnson screamed so loudly, his wife Christine rushed outside wondering what in the world was going on. She asked if he was all right. 

“We got J.T.,” Johnson said, beaming at his wife.  

Tuimoloau officially committed to the Buckeyes on July 4 and arrived on campus later that month. Two weeks into preseason camp, the praise Tuimoloau received on the recruiting trail has been verified. The 6-foot-4, 275-pound true freshman has turned plenty of heads in Columbus, leading to him shedding his black stripe only 10 practices into his Buckeye career. 

“One thing about him, he came into town in great shape,” Johnson said. “And you don’t come in and start and get where he’s going unless you’re in great shape. He’s got a really high football IQ. And so he really understands the football game. And he’s a video guy, he’s a study guy, and I think that’s why he’s starting to advance too fast is because he’s got some things that you have to have if you want to be an elite player. But his work ethic is off the charts.”

When OSU’s defensive linemen spoke to reporters Tuesday afternoon, the most popular players of discussion were two true freshmen: Tuimoloau and Jack Sawyer. 

“He’s great,” Haskell Garrett said of Tuimoloau. “He’s a student of the game. He kind of reminds me of me when I was a freshman, just trying to soak up everything and just learn. He’s just a real good student of the game and just wants to be the best person he can be on and off the field.”

That speaks to how much hype has surrounded those freshmen dating back to their recruitments. Especially considering the Buckeyes boast the likes of Zach Harrison, Tyreke Smith, Garrett, Antwuan Jackson, Javontae Jean-Baptiste and Taron Vincent, giving Ohio State’s defensive line no shortage of veteran depth and star power.

“The speed of college ball is way different than high school ball, but I think all my boys and the D-line crew is helping me, getting me up to pace and continuing to push me, make me better,” Tuimoloau told Eleven Warriors at an NIL Management event on Aug. 8. “It’s a great honor just to learn from these dudes. For them to be my big brother and always correct me if I’m doing things wrong or teach me their ways, because they’ve been doing what they’re doing. So just to be around them and to learn from them and just be good friends with them, brothers with them, I love it.”

Johnson wouldn’t commit to putting Tuimoloau or Sawyer in the Rushmen package right away, but Ryan Day said last week that Tuimoloau’s fast start to fall camp has put him in a position to earn playing time right away.

“He’s made up a lot of ground in a very short amount of time,” Day said. “He’s made a lot of plays and we’re having a hard time blocking him.”

Johnson already said this spring that he would find a role for Sawyer if Sawyer earns it, and call Johnson cautiously optimistic Tuimoloau can see the field early, too.

“I’m hoping he can,” Johnson said. “He certainly has the tools to do that. Right now it’s how fast he can learn our defense, moving forward, playing the scheme and then playing at a fast pace. People don’t understand that when you come from high school, it’s not as fast as they think it is until you hit the college campus, and now getting him up to speed. He’s starting to work on that.”

Should Tuimoloau earn an immediate spot in the defensive line rotation, Harrison won’t be surprised.  

“J.T.’s big and strong and athletic,” Harrison said. “Those are probably his biggest assets. He’s still a freshman so he’s still growing into his body, he’s got a lot of room to fill out and it’s going to be a huge asset for the Buckeyes in the next coming years.”

Read original article here

Smart foam material gives robotic hand the ability to self-repair

A robotic hand with the AiFoam artificially innervated smart foam, which enables it to sense objects in proximity by detecting their electrical fields and also self-heals if it gets cut, is pictured at National University Singapore’s Materials Sciences and Engineering lab in Singapore on June 30, 2021. (Travis Teo, Reuters)

SINGAPORE (Reuters) – Singapore researchers have developed a smart foam material that allows robots to sense nearby objects, and repairs itself when damaged, just like human skin.

Artificially innervated foam, or AiFoam, is a highly elastic polymer created by mixing fluoropolymer with a compound that lowers surface tension.

This allows the spongy material to fuse easily into one piece when cut, according to researchers at the National University of Singapore.

“There are many applications for such a material, especially in robotics and prosthetic devices, where robots need to be a lot more intelligent when working around humans,” explained lead researcher Benjamin Tee.

To replicate the human sense of touch, the researchers infused the material with microscopic metal particles and added tiny electrodes underneath the surface of the foam.

This feature enables the robotic hand to detect not only the amount but also the direction of applied force, potentially making robots more intelligent and interactive.

Tee said AiFoam is the first of its kind to combine both self-healing properties and proximity and pressure sensing. After spending over two years developing it, he and his team hope the material can be put to practical use within five years.

“It can also allow prosthetic users to have more intuitive use of their robotic arms when grabbing objects,” he said.

Reporting by Lee Ying Shan and Travis Teo; Writing by Xu Xiao; Editing by Karishma Singh and Stephen Coates

Photos

More stories you may be interested in

Read original article here

Smart foam material gives robotic hand the ability to self-repair

SINGAPORE, July 6 (Reuters) – Singapore researchers have developed a smart foam material that allows robots to sense nearby objects, and repairs itself when damaged, just like human skin.

Artificially innervated foam, or AiFoam, is a highly elastic polymer created by mixing fluoropolymer with a compound that lowers surface tension.

This allows the spongy material to fuse easily into one piece when cut, according to the researchers at the National University of Singapore.

“There are many applications for such a material, especially in robotics and prosthetic devices, where robots need to be a lot more intelligent when working around humans,” explained lead researcher Benjamin Tee.

To replicate the human sense of touch, the researchers infused the material with microscopic metal particles and added tiny electrodes underneath the surface of the foam.

When pressure is applied, the metal particles draw closer within the polymer matrix, changing their electrical properties. These changes can be detected by the electrodes connected to a computer, which then tells the robot what to do, Tee said.

“When I move my finger near the sensor, you can see the sensor is measuring the changes of my electrical field and responds accordingly to my touch,” he said.

This feature enables the robotic hand to detect not only the amount but also the direction of applied force, potentially making robots more intelligent and interactive.

Tee said AiFoam is the first of its kind to combine both self-healing properties and proximity and pressure sensing. After spending over two years developing it, he and his team hope the material can be put to practical use within five years.

“It can also allow prosthetic users to have more intuitive use of their robotic arms when grabbing objects,” he said.

Reporting by Lee Ying Shan and Travis Teo; Writing by Xu Xiao; Editing by Karishma Singh and Stephen Coates

Our Standards: The Thomson Reuters Trust Principles.

Read original article here

China is building up its ability to weaponize trade, new report says

A Chinese flag attached to the back of a boat flaps in the wind as cargo containers sit on the dock of Shenzhen Port in Shenzhen, China.

Daniel Berehulak | Getty Images News | Getty Images

China is diversifying its supply of critical natural resources — a move that will shore up Beijing’s ability to weaponize trade against its geopolitical rivals, according to a newly published report by risk consultancy Verisk Maplecroft.

“If China has a soft underbelly, it is its high dependency on foreign natural resources,” read the report released on Thursday.

China is a large consumer of major commodities including crude oil and iron ore. But the country relies heavily on imports to meet its domestic demand for those commodities.

One way the country is diversifying its import sources is by buying stakes in overseas companies, said Verisk Maplecroft. Doing that will increase the proportion of Chinese-owned resources in the country’s total imports, the report said.

As an example, the consultancy said the number of Chinese-owned base metals and gold companies in Oceania rose from zero in the year 2000 to 59 last year. It represented around 22.6% of total foreign ownership in such companies, the report showed.

Oceania is a region that includes Australia, Papua New Guinea, New Zealand, Fiji and numerous island nations.

“China is seeking to strengthen its control over global supply chains via overseas investments and partnerships with international majors. Beijing has been supporting Chinese SOEs [state-owned enterprises] to ‘go global’ and establish control of resource bases overseas since the late 1990s,” said the report.

Pivoting toward ‘autocratic regimes’

China imports critical commodities such as crude oil, natural gas, metallurgical coal and iron ore from a “highly concentrated” group of trading partners, said Verisk Maplecroft.

In diversifying its import sources, China prefers suppliers from “stable autocratic regimes” to democracies that may involve frequent changes in governments or potential shifts in policy, it added.

By securing diversified sources, China will be in a better position to weaponise trade with geopolitical rivals, while at the same time increasing the economic dependence of new and existing partners.

“Our data shows China is pivoting towards more autocratic regimes that represent greater stability for its supply lines than democracies that are, or may become, hostile to Beijing. But it is also using its massive market as a source of diplomatic leverage,” the consultancy said.

“By securing diversified sources, China will be in a better position to weaponise trade with geopolitical rivals, while at the same time increasing the economic dependence of new and existing partners,” it said.

But democratic countries dominate the production of some resources — one of them is iron ore.

China’s largest supplier of iron ore last year was Australia, an American ally that was recently hit by Beijing’s trade restrictions.

Australia shipped a monthly average of 60.86 million tonnes of iron ore to China in 2020, accounting for more than 60% of overall Chinese imports of the commodity, Refinitiv data showed.

That’s why China has been strengthening trade ties with other iron ore producers such as Brazil and Guinea, according to the Verisk Maplecroft report.

“Despite a tougher line on Beijing under President Bolsonaro, Brazil remains a priority in China’s diversification strategy, while Guinea is politically well disposed to Beijing amid democratic backslide,” the report said.

Weaponizing trade

Companies and investors are in the direct sights of Beijing’s diplomatic moves and will need to prepare accordingly.

Such geopolitical tensions could persist as China speeds up its effort to reduce its reliance on “unfriendly” resource suppliers, said Verisk Maplecroft.

“Banning imports of coal from Australia was a prime example but more are likely to follow, with significant impacts on the trade in global commodities and the geopolitical landscape likely,” it said.

“Companies and investors are in the direct sights of Beijing’s diplomatic moves and will need to prepare accordingly.”

Read original article here

A Cephalopod Has Passed a Cognitive Test Designed For Human Children

A new test of cephalopod smarts has reinforced how important it is for us humans to not underestimate animal intelligence.

Cuttlefish have been put to a new version of the marshmallow test, and the results appear to demonstrate that there’s more going on in their strange little brains than we knew.

 

Their ability to learn and adapt, the researchers said, could have evolved to give cuttlefish an edge in the cutthroat eat-or-be-eaten marine world they live in.

The marshmallow test, or Stanford marshmallow experiment, is pretty straightforward. A child is placed in a room with a marshmallow. They are told if they can manage not to eat the marshmallow for 15 minutes, they’ll get a second marshmallow, and be allowed to eat both.

This ability to delay gratification demonstrates cognitive abilities such as future planning, and it was originally conducted to study how human cognition develops; specifically, at what age a human is smart enough to delay gratification if it means a better outcome later.

Because it’s so simple, it can be adjusted for animals. Obviously you can’t tell an animal they’ll get a better reward if they wait, but you can train them to understand that better food is coming if they don’t eat the food in front of them straight away.

Some primates can delay gratification, along with dogs, albeit inconsistently. Corvids, too, have passed the marshmallow test.

 

Last year, cuttlefish also passed a version of the marshmallow test. Scientists showed that common cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) can refrain from eating a meal of crab meat in the morning once they have learnt dinner will be something they like much better – shrimp.

As a team of researchers led by behavioural ecologist Alexandra Schnell of the University of Cambridge point out in a new paper, however, in this case it’s difficult to determine whether this change in foraging behaviour in response to prey availability was also being governed by an ability to exert self-control.

So they designed another test, for six common cuttlefish. The cuttlefish were placed in a special tank with two enclosed chambers that had transparent doors so the animals could see inside. In the chambers were snacks – a less-preferred piece of raw king prawn in one, and a much more enticing live grass shrimp in the other.

The doors also had symbols on them that the cuttlefish had been trained to recognise. A circle meant the door would open straight away. A triangle meant the door would open after a time interval between 10 and 130 seconds. And a square, used only in the control condition, meant the door stayed closed indefinitely.

 

In the test condition, the prawn was placed behind the open door, while the live shrimp was only accessible after a delay. If the cuttlefish went for the prawn, the shrimp was immediately removed.

Meanwhile, in the control group, the shrimp remained inaccessible behind the square-symbol door that wouldn’t open.

The researchers found that all of the cuttlefish in the test condition decided to wait for their preferred food (the live shrimp), but didn’t bother to do so in the control group, where they couldn’t access it.

“Cuttlefish in the present study were all able to wait for the better reward and tolerated delays for up to 50-130 seconds, which is comparable to what we see in large-brained vertebrates such as chimpanzees, crows and parrots,” Schnell said.

The other part of the experiment was to test how good the six cuttlefish were at learning. They were shown two different visual cues, a grey square and a white one. When they approached one, the other would be removed from the tank; if they made the “correct” choice, they would be rewarded with a snack.

Once they had learnt to associate a square with a reward, the researchers switched the cues, so that the other square now became the reward cue. Interestingly, the cuttlefish that learnt to adapt to this change the quickest were also the cuttlefish that were able to wait longer for the shrimp reward.

That seems like cuttlefish can exert self control, all right, but what’s not clear is why. In species such as parrots, primates, and corvids, delayed gratification has been linked to factors such as tool use (because it requires planning ahead), food caching (for obvious reasons) and social competence (because prosocial behaviour – such as making sure everyone has food – benefits social species).

Cuttlefish, as far as we know, don’t use tools or cache food, nor are they especially social. The researchers think this ability to delay gratification may instead have something to do with the way cuttlefish forage for their food.

“Cuttlefish spend most of their time camouflaging, sitting and waiting, punctuated by brief periods of foraging,” Schnell said.

“They break camouflage when they forage, so they are exposed to every predator in the ocean that wants to eat them. We speculate that delayed gratification may have evolved as a byproduct of this, so the cuttlefish can optimise foraging by waiting to choose better quality food.”

It’s a fascinating example of how very different lifestyles in very different species can result in similar behaviours and cognitive abilities. Future research should, the team noted, try to determine if indeed cuttlefish are capable of planning for the future.

The team’s research has been published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

 

Read original article here

Cuttlefish have ability to exert self-control, study finds | Marine life

Humans, chimps, parrots and crows have evolved to exert self-control, a trait linked to higher intelligence. Now, researchers say cuttlefish – chunky squid-like creatures with eight arms – also have the ability to delay gratification for a better reward.

Researchers used an adapted version of the Stanford marshmallow test, in which children were given the choice of scoffing an immediate reward (one marshmallow) or waiting to earn a delayed, but better, reward (two marshmallows), on six cuttlefish in an aquarium environment.

The invertebrates were presented with chambers that were marked with different visual cues in the form of shapes. For instance, one cue meant the moment food was put in that chamber, the door would open – while another meant that when food was placed in that chamber, there would be a delay before the door opened. One of the cues was counterintuitive – even though the food was placed in the chamber, and the door was opened, there was an extra layer of plastic thwarting the cuttlefish from eating the reward.

When the cuttlefish were exposed to the chambers initially, they immediately attacked when they saw the food. Over time, they realised that each chamber had its own rules. Eventually, the molluscs didn’t even bother approaching the “unobtainable” chamber because they learned they could never secure access to the food.

With training completed, the cuttlefish were then tested in the presence of two chambers – in the “immediate” chamber they were presented with their second-preference food, while in the “delay” chamber they were given their first-preference food.

In the control setting, these conditions were mirrored, except the delay chamber was the unobtainable chamber. “We wanted to see if they were able to exert self-control in a flexible manner depending on the context,” said lead author Dr Alex Schnell, a behavioural ecologist from the University of Cambridge.

“They could see their preferred food in the unobtainable chamber, but they could never get to it – so they needed to make a decision whether to attempt to, or just take the immediate option.”

Overall, the cuttlefish delayed gratification when it led to a prey item of higher quality and were able to maintain delays for periods of up to 50 seconds to 130 seconds, the authors wrote in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

Previous research had suggested that some primates and birds exhibited this advanced level of self-control because they were social species that maintained multiple relationships and used tools, said Schnell.

These species might not forage or hunt in a particular moment so that they could build tools or wait until their partner had eaten, she noted. “But that doesn’t apply to cuttlefish … they are not social and they don’t use tools.”

Instead, self-control might have evolved in cuttlefish to maximise efficiency, she speculated. “They’re a juicy meal … so they spend really long periods camouflaged, and remain almost motionless so that they can avoid being detected by predators. And this motionless behaviour is broken up when cuttlefish forage.”

Read original article here

Touchless tech mimics the ability to ‘press’ an elevator button

In their COVID—19 information page, both the CDC and the WHO warn people against touching their faces with unwashed hands, since the virus can survive on surfaces. There’s been a rise in the use of touchless technologies as a result, and Singaporean studio Stuck Design envisions a world wherein passengers won’t even have to press elevator buttons with their fingers to get to where they need to go. The studio’s Kinetic Touchless technology can mimic the movement of one’s fingers and recreate the tactile response of pushing a button.

It works by using motion as an input method so it doesn’t need direct contact and can imitate the movement from a distance. The studio says the type of touchless interaction its technology offers can be applied to various gestures, as well. That means buttons using the technology can sink inwards or move outwards if the user makes a pushing or pulling motion. A row of buttons can also follow the user’s finger if they make a sliding movement.

Read original article here